Reference
Ward,
J., Huckstep, B., & Tsakanikos, E. (2006). Sound-colour synaesthesia: To
what extent does it use cross-modal mechanisms common to us all? Cortex, 42(2), 264-280.
(Available through UTL
catalogue)
Summary
In this study conducted at University
College London (London, UK), the nature of cross-modal perception in
synesthetes was investigated. The term
“synesthesia” is used to describe a neurological condition in which one type of
sensory stimulation evokes the automatic and involuntary sensation of another. One of the most common types of
audio-triggered synesthesia is chromesthesia, or “coloured hearing”—when the
hearing of sounds produces the automatic and involuntary visualization of colours
and patterns. A group of synesthetes who
reported colour sensations in response to music were examined alongside a
control group to find out whether this type of synesthesia employs similar mechanisms
used in normal cross-modal perception (common to all people), or whether there
are direct, privileged pathways between unimodal auditory and unimodal visual
areas that are unique to synesthetes.
Ward, Huckstep, and Tsakanikos suggest
that studies in synaesthesia can be used to inform theories of normal cognition. Chromesthesia can be especially useful in
this regard because there is evidence that suggests that not only do cross-modal
audiovisual mechanisms exist in the normal population in a more general sense,
but “coloured hearing” might be present in all of us from birth, but disappears
over time.
Some studies assume that all humans
are born with neural mechanisms capable of synesthesia, but somehow lose them
during normal development. People that do retain this neural hardware retain
synesthesia in adulthood. This view
suggests that there are special neural pathways in synesthetes that are absent
in other adults, i.e. synaesthesia uses cross-modal mechanisms that are not common to us all. Ward, Huckstep, and Tsakanikos are questioning
this opinion in their experiment. The
second hypothesis, and the one this particular study is based on, is that sound-colour
synaesthesia possibly arises from utilising pathways that are used to integrate
audio and visual stimuli as part of the normal mechanisms of cross-modal perception. There exist cross-modal audiovisual areas in
the brain that are more responsive to the combined
stimuli of sound and visuals than to either stimulus separately (e.g., during lip
reading). Because these pathways are common to us all, it is possible
that synesthesia is just an advanced mutation of these pathways.
Experiment 1 first examined whether
these sound-colour associations were arbitrary, i.e. were the colours that
synesthetes saw in relation to a specific tone same/similar to other
synesthetes, or were they drastically different? The authors first inform us that synesthetes
have not been compared to non-synesthetes in this regard before this
study. However, there are some
universals amongst non-synesthetes in terms of the sound-colour relationship,
for example: (1) most people can generate visual imagery to music on demand,
and (2) most people tend to associate higher pitch sounds with lighter colours. The results showed that sound-colour synesthetes
had a greater internal consistency of matching colours to various pitches and
timbres than the control group. However,
both groups generally used the same heuristics for matching between the audio
and visual stimuli (e.g., pitch to lightness, timbre to colour). These results confirm the hypothesis that
sound-colour synesthesia is an extension of cross-modal mechanisms common to us
all, rather than a privileged pathway between auditory and visual modalities
not present in non-synesthetes.
Experiment 2 aimed to establish that
synesthetes had automatic experiences of colour when presented with a tone. In a procedure similar to a Stroop Test, the
synesthete and control groups were asked to say the colour of a patch on the
screen while simultaneously listening to various tones which they were told to
ignore. A colour either congruent or
incongruent with the colour on the screen would be automatically generated
within the synesthetes. The results
showed that colours in synesthetes are automatically elicited to such an extent
that they are produced during a cross-modal Stroop task.
In Experiment 3, the nature of the
sound and colour were not the focus. Rather,
the synesthetes and non-synesthetes participated in a cross-modal variation of
the Posner cueing paradigm. An auditory
cue is (non-laterally) presented through headphones while simultaneously, two
coloured rectangles appear on the left and right of the screen, one of which is
synesthetically corresponding with the sound.
The task involves detecting the target, an asterisk, present in one of
the two rectangles right after the auditory cue. The results showed that the auditory cue
oriented attention to the synesthetically analogous location of the asterisk. Detection of the lateralised target was enhanced when combined with a
synesthetically congruent sound-colour pairing in both the synesthetic and control groups.
The results of these experiments
suggested that sound-colour synesthesia does indeed use similar (if not the
same) mechanisms used in normal cross-modal perception common to us all, and not special, direct, or privileged
pathways between unimodal auditory and visual pathways that are only found in
synesthetes.
Reflection
I was very excited to read this
article because of its relation to the discourse on synesthetic art. In addition to being a widely recognized
condition in neuroscience, the term “synesthesia” is used in fine art to
describe the simultaneous perception of multiple stimuli incorporated into one
gestalt experience. This can include, for example, multi-sensory projects in
the genres of visual music and sound visualization, audio-visual art, and intermedia.
The idea of “synesthetic art” can refer to either (1) art created by synesthetes
or (2) art that attempts to transmit or simulate the synesthetic experience. It
is an attempt to grasp the cognitive results of the subjective perceptual
experiences of natural synesthetes, and translate them into the realities of
non-synesthetes.
One prominent example of synesthetic
art is visual music. The techniques used
within this genre to visualize the sounds in music attempt to turn all
listener-spectators exposed to them into audio-visual synesthetes,
manually. This simulated or induced
synesthetic experience is often part of electronic dance music (EDM)
performances—probably due to their otherwise visually unexciting nature. EDM artists hire extensive teams of set
designers, artistic directors, graphic designers, and computer programmers, and
put ours of thought and consideration into creating shows that have less to do
with purely auditory experiences and become intermedia spectacles.
The technique of sound visualization
is widely used. Sound or music visualization
can be described as moving nonrepresentational imagery based on, or derived
from, the organization of sound within music. Abstract qualities found in
music—including rhythm, tempo, mood, counterpoint,
intensity, harmony, and compositional structure—are assimilated within visual phenomena.
The moving images can be generated in real time by a software program, or manually
conceptualized through computer graphics programs based on sine waves. The imagery
is synchronized with the audio as it is played in real time.
What makes this practise more intriguing
is that according to Ward, Huckstep, and Tsakanikos’s findings, the visual qualia
that the creators of any particular visualised music attribute to the source
sounds would be very similar to the visual qualia that any spectator-listener would
attribute to the same sounds. The colour-to-timbre or brightness-to-pitch
associations are not all completely arbitrary amongst non-synesthetes (although
they are a lot less consistent than in true synesthetes, of course), and prove
to be significantly similar. This
suggests a certain level of intersubjectivity amongst the audio-visual perceptive
experience. This hypothesis has value in
that the simulated audio-visual synesthetic experience seen as intense or
meaningful by its creators would be received as equally intense or meaningful by
its perceivers, and would simultaneously cultivate a more involved and significant
relationship with that artist’s music.
No comments:
Post a Comment