“If music be the food of love, play on.”- William
Shakespeare
Evolution
is a highly efficient process. Every aspect of our physiology, and often our
behaviour, has an advantage for survival. Even the appendix, thought to be a
vestigial accessory of the intestine, has involvement in probiotic maintenance1.
With that in mind, gaining the ability to not only hear, but to cognitively
process music requires a phenomenal biological investment, as outlined in
multiple chapters of Robert Jourdain’s “Music, the Brain, and Ecstasy.” Jourdain
elaborates (pg 307-308) on the theory that music developed for social
interaction, and in order to solidify social bonds or mediate conflict. This argument
is logical; however, language developed for mostly the same reasons, and is
equally, if not more efficient in allowing social interaction and settling
conflict. Evolution is a never-ending balancing act between the benefit of a
new capability against the energy cost of producing it, and a secondary method
of social bonding, or happenstance effect from language processing, seems by far
too weak a motive to maintain a cognitive system that is specific and elaborate
enough to experience music. There has to be a better reason, and in this essay
I will argue that music increases the human capacity to experience and express
love. For these purposes, the term love will encompass affection for offspring,
partners, and towards community members. I will begin by outlining why love is a
biological necessity that is important enough to drive the evolution of music
appreciation, and then briefly describe from a neural plasticity standpoint, how
activating the circuitry involved in music may enhance the experience of love.
There is no doubt that a loving
environment fosters the healthy psychological development of children, but
recently there has been a linked made between this benefit and BDNF (brain
derived neurotrophic factor)2. BDNF is a protein that is broadly
expressed in the central nervous system3, and serves to enhance the
survival of neurons and promote neurogenesis4. In children that have
wanted for cuddling and love, this gene will be permenantly down-regulated, and in women it will also affect the
BDNF expression of their offspring2,4. Clinically, down-regulated
BDNF causes susceptibility to major depression, bipolar-disorder, and schizoprenia2,
while normal expression supports learning, memory, and stress managment4.
Clearly, love is of vital importance in early brain development, and if music can
help facilitate the expression of love in a community or towards offspring,
this would offer a strong advantage. Later in life, love and the ability to
express and experience deep connections with others, become crucial for success
within a group, and for finding a mate to raise children with. Emotions in
general influence motivation (pg 311) and decision making (pg 309), but love is
unique in that the expression and experience thereof can directly impact one’s
ability to mate and the viability of those offspring. In support of the evolutionary intention of
music appreciation, consider Jourdain’s description of the cognitive complexities
of melodic anticipation (chapter 3), and sense of meter (chapter 5). The
ability to distinguish animal calls and hear is of clear advantage (pg 2-3),
but melodic and rhythmic appreciation have no evident impact on human survival;
yet, these abilities have been conserved to varying degrees in almost every
member of the human race (pg 286), and illicit an emotional response unlike
that of any visual stimulus (pg 328). Jourdain mentions (pg 308) explicitly
that music somehow, undeniably embodies emotion, and if love is so critical to
our survival, a system designed to enhance our experience of it would be worth
the biological investment.
The old
adage, “practice makes perfect,” comes to mind when Jourdain explains how music
practice reinforces the neural pathways required for performance, while neglect
degrades them (pg 225). This is an example of synaptic plasticity, which is the
activity-based change in synapse-mediated connectivity between neurons5.
Essentially, the more you use a synaptic pathway, the stronger it becomes. If
the experiences of music or love overlap in neural circuitry, exposure to one
could theoretically strengthen the response and plasticity of the other. In support of this theory, Jourdain mentions
that music pleasure is associated with endorphin release (pg 327), which is
also implicated in feelings of love6; although this does not prove
identical neuronal circuitry, it suggests similarity based on release of the
same neurotransmitters. In addition, music, much like love (it is better to
have loved and lost than to never have loved at all), has the ability to make
sad experiences seem dignified or “worth it” (pg322). This emotional phenomenon
is highly unique to both love and music, and may imply similar neurological
pathways.
Much
like the theory of muscular representation (pg 325), the conclusion that music
might increase the capacity to experience and express love is highly
speculative. However, there is currently no satisfying rationale as to why
human beings evolved the ability to appreciate music, and given the
significance of love in our development and survival, this would be a strong evolutionary
incentive. In addition, there are anecdotal and hormonal indications that the
experiences of love and music share similar neurological pathways. Oscar Wilde felt that music could propel an
individual to emotional intensities beyond their life experiences; while
Jourdain writes that the meaning evoked in music is only what one personally
brings to it (pg 322). I argue that the truth is somewhere in between, and that
if the experience of love and music are not neurologically similar, it is
uncanny how comparable they are in creating a unique sense of ecstasy.
(Word count: 919, excluding references)
References
1.
Bollinger, R.R. et al 2007 Journal of Theoretical Biology 249(4): 826-831
2.
Roth, T.L. et
al 2009 Biological Psychiatry 65(9): 760-769
3.
Conner, J.M. et
al 1997 Journal of Neuroscience 17(7):2295-2313
4.
Sullivan, R. and Lasley, E.N. 2010 Cerebrum
17:1-13
5.
Ho, V.M. et al 2011 Science 334:623-628.
6.
Hawkes, C.H. 1992 Journal of Neurology,
Neurosurgery, and Neuropsychiatry 55:247-250
No comments:
Post a Comment