Article:
Davis, J. I. (2012). Four Applications of Embodied Cognition. Topics
in Cognitive Science, 4(4), 786–793.
This
article discusses the concepts of embodied cognition from four view
points by seven different authors. These viewpoints discuss embodied
cognition (how the body and environment shape the mind) and its
relation to or influence on our understanding of the legal system,
art and literature, architecture and music cognition. For this blog
post I will discuss 2 of these views: 1) Literature and Art and 2)
Music Cognition. While the first view does not have to do with music,
it provides some interesting points in regard to literature and art.
1)
Embodiment In Literature and Visual Art - by Ellen Esrock
Esrock
discusses how reading and viewing has traditionally been seen as
"fundamentally non-bodily". She discusses how recent
scholarship on embodied cognition now considers how the body is
involved in the acts of reading and viewing art. Esrock starts the
article with a great example of how we might have a "bodily
response" to what we read or view. For example, when reading
about a streamstress' hand sewing, we may "feel" physical
tension in our hands. Or when viewing a piece of art depicting a
woman embroidering, we may "feel' the fabric or the needle
moving through the fabric. Esrock says that this experience of
feeling what we read or view is called "transomatization",
as sort of "bodily immersion" into the text or piece of
art. It's as if we mimic what we read or view.
Esrock
goes on to discuss how embodied cognition looks at emotions as
"bodily". She says that current studies are looking at
empathy in literature and art - how it is that we can be have an
emotional response when reading about or viewing a depiction of an
emotional moment. Esrock goes on to say that literary and visual
studies of embodied cognition, which examine areas of "human
throughout, emotion and behaviour", may be helpful to other
disciplines. She concludes by saying that teaching "embodied"
subjects how to read and view literature/art may help them develop
"cognitive and affective skills in other areas of life".
2)
Embodied Music Cognition - by Leon van Noorden and Marc Leman
Noorden
and Leman start their article by saying that "embodied music
cognition sees music experience as based on perception and action".
They discuss the idea that movement and music are intertwined and
that in many cultures music and dance are not considered separate
arts. The authors discuss that it is through movement that people may
find meaning in music. This contrasts with traditional approaches to
music cognition, which look at musical meaning from a "disembodied"
approach - one that is only based on perception with little
consideration of how the mind, body and environment are
interconnected. Noorden and Leman say that ongoing research is now
interested in how the human body is implicated in the creation of
meaning in music. They go on to discuss a few interesting examples of
this.
Noorden
and Leman mention that embodied music cognition may help us
understand how music impacts social interactions. They discuss recent
studies that showed that children move more synchronously with music
when they dance in a group, as opposed to when they dance
individually. They also discuss the phenomenon of "resonant
perception-action coupling", which occurs around the tempo
frequency of 2 Hz (120 bpm). Studies have observed how resonance
around 2 Hz causes changes in walking. Studies have also shown that
children around 3 - 4 years of age can only synchronize with music
when it is played at around 2 Hz. Other studies have also shown that
2Hz is best frequency for rocking babies to sleep. Interestingly,
Noorden and Leman explain that by 5 years of age and older, children
start to synchronize at more varied (faster and slower) tempi than 2
Hz. The authors state that it is as if "the older children learn
to put brakes on the resonator".
Noorden
and Leman also discuss how technology has made use of the concept of
embodied music cognition. They discuss how the program DJogger, used
on personal music players, offers digital music that matches the
tempo of one's walking or running. The assumption is that
synchronizing one's walking or running to music is motivating and
stimulating (perhaps this synchronizing induces a sense of flow when
running or walking, making it easier to coordinate movements or
continue the activity for a longer period of time?). Noorden and
Leman mention another example of technology using the concept of
embodied cognition. The Sync-in Team game uses synchronization and
entrainment in a "social music interaction game". They
don't elaborate on this point, but they do say that programs such as
these were shown to create a sense of "presence and flow".
Noorden
and Leman conclude their article by commenting on how embodied music
cognition may change our understanding of music and meaning. Instead
of the traditional approach of focussing on how meaning is derived
from a "perception-based" analysis of musical content,
embodied music cognition looks at how musical meaning is formed
through perception and action. For example, many people move when
they hear music - this is a way in which we derive meaning in music.
Noorden and Leman conclude their article by saying that embodied
music cognition may impact how we understand social cognition, by
using "concepts of movement and emotion synchronicity or
entrainment".
Response:
I
thought these two articles were a good introduction to the concept of
embodied cognition. I thought the idea that we "feel" what
we read or view to be very interesting. I can't say I have every felt
powerful emotions when reading or viewing art, but I can definitely
say I have been moved to tears many times when viewing films;
however, I think a large part of this may be due to the music that
accompanies the visuals in film. It would be interesting to re-watch
a film I have had an emotional response to before, but without the
music. I wonder if I would have the same reaction, or if it is music
that is mostly responsible for my emotional response.
In
the second article, I found the idea that we can find meaning in
music through movement to be quite interesting. I find that I have a
preference for music that has a "groove" to it. Sometimes
it feels as though I cannot help but bob my head or feel like I'm
almost part of the music. I feel this even more-so when I actually
play music with a groove. I suppose this has to do with the concept
of entrainment and synchronicity, where that sense of "flow"
or "presence" occurs. Nooden and Leman mention how
programs like Djogger create this sense of "presence and flow".
I think the idea of jogging to music that matches your speed is a
great idea. I certainly have a very hard time jogging to music when
I'm not matching it's beat. I wonder if this is my musician brain
reacting or if we all have a natural impulse to synchronize with a
beat (?).
Overall,
I thought these articles were interesting overviews of the concept of
embodied cognition. However, I thought there were some ideas that
both articles could have elaborated on. One concept I would have
liked to know more about is the Sync-in Team game and exactly how it
uses entrainment in a "social music interaction game". I
also would have appreciated more background on how embodied music
cognition can help us understand social cognition.
Can
anyone share any knowledge or thoughts on this topic?
1 comment:
I found reading the second article “Embodied Music Cognition” by Leon van Noorden and Marc Leman to be especially interesting. I have recently been exposed to this topic through my piano lessons and have tested the use of movement with my repertoire. I experienced this “embodied music cognition” when I would dance around the room, while singing a particular phrase inside my head. My findings were that each phrase had more clarity in their direction and overall shape. In the book Music, Motor Control and the Brain, the authors explain in the chapter “musical synchronization” how the concept of sensorimotor synchronization (SMS) is linked to the “anticipation tendency.” The example of tapping with a metronome is used, where the taps often “precede the tones by a few tens of milliseconds.” When I was dancing certain steps to my musical phrase, my body, like the hand tapping, was anticipating the next step through my audiation. Evidently, this led to my phrase becoming more fluid and with direction when I came back to the piano because my body physically anticipated the location of the following notes. This concept has also been used in rehabilitating the gait pattern of stroke patients where they stop their steps and do not anticipate the next. When a certain rhythmic beat is played, the patients’ bodies feel the urge to match the beat and thus begin a continuos walking pattern. (68)” Thaut et al. hypothesized that there is a direct pathway from the auditory system to spinal motor neurons, which enables rhythmic input to drive motor action.” ( 68)
References:
Altenmuller E, Kesslering J, Wiesendanger M, Music, Motor Control and the Brain, Oxford University Press
Post a Comment