Effect
of Color-Coded Notation on Music Achievement of Elementary Instrumental
Students
George
Rogers, Journal of Research in Music Education, January 1991, Volume 39, Issue
1, p. 64 - 73
This research study examined the use of color-coded notation
on 92 grade five and six beginning band students. The goal of the study was to
determine if using color-coded music would have any effect on the students’ ability
to memorize music, sight-read or identify letter name notes. Divided into two
groups from two different schools, the experimental group was given color-coded
materials and traditional method books where the notes had been highlighted in different
colors. The control group used the same curriculum, but non-colored. The
results were interesting. Color-coding seemed to have no effect on the students’
ability to memorize music, but for sight reading, and in particular note naming,
the research study did show that color-coding had an effect on a higher degree
of test score accuracy. In addition to those results, the study highlighted the
effect of color-coding on a special needs class and showed that it played a
significant role in note identification. The study also revealed that 65% of students said
that using color-coded notation made it easier for them to play. At the time
this research study was undertaken, there had been no research done in color-coding in music education.
Color-coding as an educational tool has been well
documented. The paper cites several research studies that showed using color-codes
in numeracy and literacy materials improved performance results and test
scores. According to Rogers, the brain responds well to color as a learning
tool because “stimuli received through several senses excites more neurons in
several localized areas of the cortex, thereby reinforcing the learning process
and improving retention”. In other words, the brain gets excited by colors and this
results in better learning.
In the beginning band classroom the ability to read simple
notation and letter name notes is often challenging; especially for the special
needs child. The black and white language of sticks and circles
squeezed into a five line staff can be overwhelming to students who don’t
process information as quickly and efficiently as others. Color-coding, as the
study reveals, has been used over the years in various subject areas. Students take for granted the colorful Venn
diagrams and graphs of math and science text books. I
remember being engaged by colorful math textbooks from the 70’s and the colored letters of the
alphabet on Sesame Street. It seems logical that
colors applied to numbers or letters could also be a beneficial pedagogical aid
in music and score reading. If B on the
third line of the staff is pink but A on the space below is orange, one would think that at the very least, students would be able to identify and understand
that these are two different notes, and therefore, two different sounds.
In my own work with learning differentiated students, I am using numbers to identify the notes of the B flat scale. I am intrigued by the possibility that using colors has the potential to reinforce the different notes, particularly if I use color in the early stages and continue to use color as I transition to the actual note names. In the study, special needs students did well when they were tested with color-coded notes, and became highly dependent on this system. However, they resisted being tested with non-colored notation, some refusing to even try. This raises concerns that color coding and in extension, non-traditional score curriculum could have a negative impact that might restrict or impair score reading as students advance to higher grades in music education.
In my own work with learning differentiated students, I am using numbers to identify the notes of the B flat scale. I am intrigued by the possibility that using colors has the potential to reinforce the different notes, particularly if I use color in the early stages and continue to use color as I transition to the actual note names. In the study, special needs students did well when they were tested with color-coded notes, and became highly dependent on this system. However, they resisted being tested with non-colored notation, some refusing to even try. This raises concerns that color coding and in extension, non-traditional score curriculum could have a negative impact that might restrict or impair score reading as students advance to higher grades in music education.
Questions that were raised for me after reading the study include; What influence did using two different teachers for each group of students have on the final results? How did the application of “coloring in” the notes in a regular method book influence the learning? Would color-coding work more effectively on curriculum if it were designed specifically to be used in this way?
The final summary of the study reveals that no significant benefit was
found in using color-coded notation. However, recommendations do include the
potential of using color-coded materials with special needs students. It
does highlight the result that 65% of students believed color-coded notation helped them play better compared to
non-colored. I think this shows potential and leaves me wondering
if there is value in taking another look at using color-coding in music
education under different research parameters.