The origins of musical talent have been
researched and examined for centuries. From the earliest debates of Plato and
Aristotle to Carl Seashore’s testing of innate giftedness at the turn of the
century. Neuroscientists measure the sensory experience of audiation, musical
processing and brain capacities while psychologists research patterns of
behaviour and observe musical ability. The debate often focuses on talent as an
innate inheritance versus talent as experiential development. Can the definition of "talent" be accessible
and equitable to every music student in the public education classroom, including the differentiated learner?
For Bach and Hayden musical talent was
something that was gifted from above. Chosen by God, they were ordained
miraculously with a virtuosic ability. This impression of musical genius has
stayed with us through the centuries. Jourdain describes it as “the phenomenon
of inspiration”; supporting the idea that creative geniuses have something
innately special about them that sets them apart. Malcolm Gladwell has a very
simple answer about where genius comes from; 10,000 hours of practice.
In
his best-seller, Outliers, The Story of Success, Gladwell examines
“musical genius”, and cites a research study done in the early 1990’s by
psychologist K. Anders Ericsson to support his theory that talent is a result
of many hours of practice. The study examines the musical success and
virtuosity of violin players at Berlin’s Academy of Music. Players are
divided into three groups: the virtuosos/“world-class performers”; solid players
who are considered to be very good, and musicians who were “never going to play
professionally” but were planning on pursuing music pedagogy. (Gladwell, 32).
All students in the first group had
acquired at least 10,000 + hours of practice. In comparison the musicians in
the two other groups had practiced about 2,000 hours. What Ericsson discovered
was that the virtuosos not only worked harder than everyone else, they actually
worked much, much harder (Gladwell, 39). And this brings Gladwell to his magic
number. He quotes neurologist Daniel Levitin “The emerging picture from such
studies is that ten thousand hours of practice is required to achieve the level
of mastery associated with being a world class expert in anything.” Levitin
explains that it takes the brain this much time to master any subject, and
“true mastery” of the subject = 10,000 hours.
What makes Gladwell’s argument even
more compelling is that in Ericsson’s study of the violin students there were
no “naturals that emerged”. No student magically achieved greatness
effortlessly, by some innate gift or talent. All had the enormous amount of
10,000 hours of practice behind them. In addition, the study did not uncover
any “grinds”; students who had put in that amount of time and had not been
successful. Gladwell’s simple theory challenges the idea that talent is simply
innate and “God-given”.
Jourdain’s explanation of musical
talent is a little more complex. Jourdain’s list includes: superb
neurology for music, a high overall IQ, thorough training, limitless
encouragement, the right kind of personality, drive, courage, rebelliousness,
and, the luck of being born at the right time, in the right place (Jourdain,
186). Jourdain admits that there is no evidence that the brains of musical
geniuses are any different from the average brain (187). In fact, while
Jourdain acknowledges that Einstein’s brain showed double the amount of neuron
support cells, he also acknowledges that his genius may just have been as
easily attributable to hard work.
Both Jourdain and Gladwell eventually
conclude that practice, day in and day out, especially at a young age, does not
happen without encouragement; dedicated parental or guardian support, expensive
music lessons, wealth to provide those lessons and opportunities where students
will be mentored; perhaps placed in musical ensembles where that amount of
practice is tantamount. Gladwell attributes talent to a worker bee
mentality. Jourdain returns to the mysticism of “talent”, some people just seem
to have it, some people don’t.
Between logging in 10,000 hours of
practice and hoping to be a “have” instead of a “have not” where does
that leave the differentiated learner? If Gladwell’s theory proves to be
correct, that it is just a matter of 10.000 hours’ worth of practice, then
talent is attainable; talent is equitable. But it’s the fine print that
concerns me. The supportive parent, the moneyed family, the private music
lessons, the ways and means of navigating a moneyed culture when a student is raised outside that culture.
The music educator’s problem with both
of these theoretical views of talent is that it remains out of reach for the
average learner in public education. Outside of having a tenacious parent who
knows how to advocate for her own child’s ability, chances are a “virtuoso”
without economic means or educated parents will not have the opportunity to
develop their talent on an instrument. Studies show that students who are
struggling in school as identified special needs learners often share
socio-economic challenges that put them outside the “talent zone” (Fitzgerald,
2011).
In Kindling the Spark: Recognizing and
Developing Musical Talent, the author, Joanne Haroutounian broadens the scope
and definition of musical talent “that seeks to unveil potential in every
classroom.” “Talent” being a possibility
for every child. She emphasizes that
discovering this spark and nurturing it in the classroom depends on a person's
individual perspective of what constitutes musical talent.
Studies have shown that music teachers
are reluctant to work with special needs children unless they have had some
prior training or experience actually teaching them. (Gerrity et al.,2013,
Evans et al. 2011) This perspective in music education is particularly damaging
in the learning differentiated classroom. When the field of musical psychology and
music education equates musical talent with innate or inherited giftedness (Howe,
Sloboda, 1998) the development of talent in the classroom suffers. (Gerrity,
2013)
Researchers in expertise performance
have focused on reconstructing this definition of talent towards the idea of
deliberate practice. (Haroutounian,
2002) This is echoed by Gladwell’s theory of musical genius which suggests
that “talent” is accessible to all through hard work. It is only useful for the
music educator to question the origin of musical talent if the answer provides
a foundation to advocate for the equity of music education for every student
and as a fundamental component of the public education curriculum.
Children with potential talent need
opportunities to allow it to develop. Music educators need to continue an
active dialogue to discover and nurture this potential as well as to challenge all
students to develop their talents in instrumental music in the public education
classroom.
References
Evans, R. J., Bickel, R., & Pendarvis, E.
D. (2000). Musical talent: Innate or acquired?
Perceptions of students, parents,
and teachers. Gifted Child
Quarterly, 44(2),
80-90.
Fitzpatrick, K. R. (2011). A mixed methods
portrait of urban instrumental music teaching. Journal
of Research in Music Education, 59(3), 229-256.
Gerrity, K. W., Hourigan, R. M., & Horton,
P. W. (2013). Conditions That Facilitate Music Learning Among Students With
Special Needs: A Mixed-Methods Inquiry. Journal of Research in Music Education, 0022429413485428.
Gladwell,
M. (2008). Outliers: The story of success. Penguin UK.
Happé, F., & Vital, P. (2009). What aspects
of autism predispose to talent?.Philosophical
Transactions
of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences,364(1522), 1369-1375.
Haroutounian, J. (2002). Kindling
the spark: Recognizing and developing musical talent. Oxford University Press.
Hourigan, R., & Hourigan, A. (2009). Teaching music to
children with autism: understandings and
perspectives. Music Educators Journal, 40-45.
Howe, M. J., Davidson, J. W., & Sloboda, J.
A. (1998). Innate talents: Reality or
myth?. Behavioral and brain sciences, 21(03), 399-407.
Jourdain, R. (1997). Music,
the brain, and ecstasy: How music captures our imagination. W. Morrow.
Norton, A., Winner, E., Cronin, K., Overy, K., Lee, D. J.,
& Schlaug, G. (2005). Are
there
pre-existing neural, cognitive, or motoric markers for musical ability?.Brain
and cognition, 59(2),
124-134.
Obler, L. K., & Fein, D. E. (1988). The exceptional brain:
Neuropsychology of talent and special
abilities.
Guilford Press.
Ruthsatz, J., Detterman, D., Griscom, W. S.,
& Cirullo, B. A. (2008). Becoming an expert in the
musical
domain: It takes more than just practice. Intelligence,36(4),
330-338.
2 comments:
Hi Susan,
This post was really interesting! Your question/title is interesting because puts Gladwell's entire theory into question. Can ANYONE who puts in 10,000 hours into ANYTHING become an expert in it? I don't necessarily agree with "the 10,000 hour rule" because I think there is more to mastering a craft than just 10,000 hours of practice. There needs to be some sort of innate ability in addition to the practice in order to make the practice worth while.
Students with learning disabilities, I think, would have a tougher time achieving virtuosity after 10,000 hours versus those who don't have learning disabilities. Depending on the LD, it can limit one's ability and one's potential, making it much harder for them to achieve mastery.
Hello Susan, your posting is very interesting. As you mentioned, debates about gifted and learned musical abilities have existed from Plato’s time and it was a question that I asked myself for longtime since I began playing music very late and I wasn’t sure whether I had enough talent to become a professional musician or not. (I don’t ask the question to myself anymore because it is too late to convert to another field!)
I would like to recommend a book for you. The Natural Musician: “On Abilities, Giftedness and Talent”: The author, Dina Kirnarskaya is professor of Psychology and Musicology at Russian Gnesisns’ Academy of Music in Moscow. The author explains the mystery of genius by decoding stereotypes about music such as the absolute pitch and musical wunderkinds. The book consists of six chapters which cover subjects from general intellectual abilities to the relationship between music and social communication. It explains something beyond the theory of 10,000 hour practice. There are also many contexts written in neuroscience perspectives in this book. I believe the last chapter of this book “Homo Musicus” inspires all of those who work in music fields.
Post a Comment