Fisher, C., &
Larner, A. (2008). Jean
Langlais (1907–91): an historical case of a blind organist with stroke-induced
aphasia and Braille alexia but without amusia. Journal of Medical Biography, 16 (4), 232-234. Retrieved from http://jmb.rsmjournals.com.myaccess.library.utoronto.ca/content/16/4/232.full.pdf+html
Summary
This article reports a case of aphasia without amusia. It tells the story of Jean Langlais, a blind
organist who suffered from aphasia
and Braille alexia, after his stroke
in 1984 at the age of 77.
Jean
Langlais lost his sight between age 2 and 3.
He learned to read Braille at age 10, including Braille for music which
uses some of the same patterns as Braille for language. In other words, the same pattern in Braille,
depending on the context, may represent letters or musical notation. Jean Langlais was a very accomplished
musician. He was a celebrated
recitalist, composer, and noted improviser who won first prize at the Conservatory
in Paris in 1930. One of his signature
compositional techniques is his use of name
motifs in which names and even sentences become a musical theme.
His
stroke at the age of 77 affected Wernicke’s area in particular, causing him to
be diagnosed with Wernicke-like aphasia.
After his stroke, comprehension of simple questions, orders and familiar
voices was still intact, but understanding more complex orders and speaking
letter names was impossible. He could, however, sing the letter names. Jean Langlais was diagnosed with Braille agraphia
which means he had extensive difficulty writing and spelling. Interestingly, all his musical abilities
seemed to still be intact. He could
still sing a melody, execute scales and modulations, and he could still both decode
and play from musical Braille notation and compose music in Braille, often
still incorporating name motifs in post-stroke pieces. From this evidence, the author draws the
conclusion that the functional and anatomical mental processes of language and
music must be independent.
Response
I find it interesting that
there is such an overlap between notation for music and language in
Braille. Both this overlap and Jean
Langlais’ interest in name motifs would seem to indicate a strong connection
between music and language notation in Langlais’ everyday life. Therefore, it is surprising to me that he
would lose so much ability in speech and language and still retain such strong
abilities in composition and musical perception. However, Aniruddh D. Patel, who wrote the
book Music, Language, and the Brain
(2008), points out that musicians’ brains are actually quite different from those
of non-musicians. Therefore, it does not
necessarily work to make a generalized statement about music and language processing
since these processes probably relate differently for musicians and
non-musicians. Patel also states that
all reported cases of aphasia without amusia have been in musicians. It would be interesting to study aphasia in
non-musicians to see if there is any decrease in ability to process music for
these individuals. Since music activates
so many different areas of the brain, it is more likely that a musician, following
damage to a specific area of the brain, would still retain the ability to
process music, even if language and music processing abilities are somehow
connected in the brain.
No comments:
Post a Comment