This essay
examines the brain processes involved in and elicited by African Drumming class
at the University of Toronto. Rhythm is normally considered as ‘of the body’,
but according to Robert Jourdain (1997), it is very much of the brain. I started
my African Drumming class at the university with this misconception, but was
subsequently baffled by the processes involved in the difficulties and
successes I was experiencing. I asked myself questions such as “Why is this so
difficult?” and “How can I understand and execute this rhythm better?” These
are the questions that led me to a bigger question: “What’s happening in my
brain during drumming class?” With the help of Jourdain’s (1997) book, I have
explored the issue of lateralization, the question of where in my brain the
rhythm processing happens, my body’s response to these processes, how sensory
and memory systems work together and how the learning of the rhythms takes
place. These explorations lead to the inference that the understanding and
performance of rhythm is not limited to one part of the body or one section of
the brain.
Recently,
research surfaced suggesting the strict lateralization of most brain functions.
Jourdain (1997) disagrees with this theory wholeheartedly, saying that brain
function is too complex to be limited to one section or hemisphere. This, he
says, is especially true of rhythm. Generally speaking rhythm is considered to
be a left brain affair, while harmony resides on the right. Michael H. Thaut
(2005) is also an advocate of this theory, referencing a study that shows that
pitch is processed separately from rhythm (Peretz and Kolinsky 1993) and
suggesting that rhythm is bilateral. Jessica A. Grahn (2009) has also conducted
research in favor of rhythm being less localized than pitch. Interestingly,
patients who suffer left brain damage generally do not lose their rhythmic
ability, but right brain damage can eliminate harmonic skills. This suggests
that rhythm employs multiple types of cognition, and that its processes are far
less localized than those of tonality.
Jourdain
(1997) is generally less focused on rhythm in his book than he is on pitch, but
he does outline areas of the brain where rhythm is involved. Newer research
supports many of his findings, but some researchers are of the opinion that it
is challenging to assign a particular function to a specific area of the brain (Grahn
2009).
Premotor Cortex – This is the part of the brain that becomes
active just before the motor cortex. Thaut (2005) attributes this section to
planning, voluntary control and movement execution. Grahn (2009), in her study,
also mentions the involvement of the Supplementary Motor area in these
functions.
Basal Ganglia – This primitive part of the brain manages
lengthy sequences of adjustments of posture and is involved in action selection
and learning.
Cerebellum – Balance is one of the main functions of this
section of the brain but it also harmonizes the motions of the body. Thaut
(2005) mentions that the cerebellum integrates sensory and motor information.
This, and the fact that the cerebellum is a muscle coordinator suggests that
this area of the brain may play an important role in proper instrumental
technique.
Significantly,
these three areas of the brain are closely connected. Research also shows that
the arms and hands are controlled by two different parts of the brain. The
cerebellum controls hand details while the basal ganglia controls the arms. It
is fascinating to consider how these two systems are able to work together in
the motions of African Drumming.
Parietal Cortex – “Assembles incoming sensations into maps of the
body and its environment.” (Jourdain 1997 p. 217) The left parietal cortex
sequences motions on both sides of the body. There are many neurons here that
are specific to the arms and hands. This fact leads us into the investigation
of how these neural processes manifest themselves in the body.
Generally,
one thinks of rhythm as a bodily experience. For example, one might think of
themselves as bodily (or culturally) predisposed to African Drumming, or not.
Jourdain (1997) suggests that the coordination of limbs is not of the body, but
of the brain, and that they are intermediated by the cerebral cortex. He illustrates
this idea beautifully when he says “when people insist that rhythm comes from
the body, they are really thinking about the pleasure they gain by representing
rhythm in their motor systems.” (p. 149)
Although
movement involves the entire brain, the motor cortex is lateralized. The
volition of performance has been identified in previous sections, but what of
the physical execution? The hands, one of the few parts of the body with very
fine motor skills (another part is the face) have a disproportionately bigger
chunk of motor cortex assigned to them. Additionally, Jourdain (1997) claims
that there is no muscle that works alone, so beating a sogo rhythm in drumming class would include many more parts of the
body than just the hands. Again, this supports the theory that rhythm and
rhythm production cannot be confined to one section of the brain, or the body
for that matter.
Then,
finally, is the question of how learning takes place in the drumming class.
There is no notation to follow and rhythms are expected to be memorized. How is
this achieved in such a unique learning environment? From my own observation, I
noticed different types of memory at work. I feel the drum under my fingers as
I hit (tactile), I hear what the correct rhythm is supposed to sound like
(auditory) and I watch the teacher’s hand movements carefully (visual).
Jourdain (1997) describes performance as a never ending cycle of feedback and
adjustment, and this is certainly true of African Drumming. My aim is truly to
make the different beats into what Jourdain (1997) calls a property of the
motor system. Thaut (2005) views this as a type of brain plasticity or
entrainment. What he claims is that after repeated practice and experience, new
synaptical networks emerge in the brain. Miell, Macdonald and Hargreaves (2005)
claim that “Musical rhythm rapidly creates stable and precise internal
templates for temporal organization of motor responses” (p. 184).
For me,
being an auditory learner, the most helpful technique was the verbalization of
rhythms. Thaut (2005) cites research that suggests that the motor system is
sensitive to arousal by auditory cues and that this process happens without the
individual being cognizant of the process. Perhaps this is why the internal
sound of my drumming teacher saying jiki
jiki jang jiki chaan jiki jang is a sure fire way for me to remember the
particular drum beat associated with those syllables.
This brief
investigation has led me to the conclusion that my brain is extremely active
during drumming class. Not only is my brain processing what I hear and see, but
it is eliciting appropriate responses from my body in a performance
environment. This experience involves all parts of the body, but it seems that likewise,
this rhythmic experience cannot be limited to one part of the brain.
References
Grahn, J.
A. (2009). Neuroscientific Investigations of Musical Rhythm: Recent Advances
and Future Challenges. Contemporary Music
Review, 28 (3), 251-277
Jourdain, R
(1997). Music, the Brain and Ecstasy. How
Music Captures our Imagination. New York, NY: HarperCollins Publishers.
Miell, D.,
Macdonald, R., & Hargreaves, D. J. (2005). Musical Communication. New York: Oxford University Press.
Thaut, M.
H. (2005). Rhythm, Music and the Brain. Scientifc Foundations and Clinical
Applications. New York, NY: Routledge
2 comments:
Very interesting read Reanna! I find it interesting how we always associate rhythm with the body and yet our brain is obviously so much more involved than we give it credit for - I would say that we probably hold many of the same misconceptions about different kinds of dance. I feel like a possible cause for this bodily association might be the same as Jourdain's description of the virtuosic musician who is thinking about music in terms that are more over-arching and less structural - the people who describe rhythm as "in the body" have it so hard-wired brain-wise that they aren't actively thinking about the movements/thought-processes involved.
(Also ignore my deleted comment, no idea why that happened!)
Post a Comment