“How many of you studied with music in the
background during your undergraduate studies? And how many of you studied in
silence?” asked one of the graduate school orientation organizers. The question
was proposed in order to make the point that we all made it to the same place
despite having a variety of studying and music listening routines. During my
undergraduate studies, I enjoyed upbeat background music during tasks that used
up less of my “brain power” and needed silence to comprehend complex concepts. Songs
I liked tempted me to sing along and soft music lowered my energy level and led
me to sleep. My interest in how well our brain copes with dual processes, music
and learning tasks, led me to complete this short essay on how the performance
of three types of tasks usually involved in studying (attention, recall,
comprehension) correlate with varying background music conditions. The sources
chosen to be reviewed were limited to those published after Robert Jourdain’s
book “Music, the Brain and Ecstasy”, not relating to learning tasks other than
the ones mentioned above, and whose
participants were between 19-30 years old.
Attention
tasks
Attention
task performance was shown to vary based on the presence or absence of lyrics
and the music preferences of the participants. In Shi, Huang and Chiang (2012)
and Huang and Shih (2011), identification of the number of asterisks in a
series of scrambled codes was used as the test for attention.
Shi,
Huang and Chiang (2012) based their research on three conditions – quiet, music
with lyrics, and music without lyrics. There was a significant negative impact
on the attention performance in the music with lyrics environment compared to
the quiet environment. There was no significant difference between quiet and
music without lyrics or between music with lyrics and music without lyrics. The music with lyrics and music without lyrics
did not differ in volume or tune. Details were not available as to which
instrument would carry the tune instead of the voice. The findings of this
research, that the presence of words diverts one’s attention from the task,
supports Jourdain’s (1997) idea of the importance of words to melody listening.
Huang
and Shih (2011) found that participants with no background music scored higher
on an attention test than participants exposed to popular songs, classical
light music or traditional Chinese music. Although popular songs, the only
category that may have contained lyrics, did not significantly differ from the
classical light (instrumental) and the traditional Chinese (instrumental), I
noticed that the mean score was slightly lower than the instrumental
categories.
It seems that a quiet environment produced
better attention scores than music with lyrics in both papers, although not
quite significantly better in the Huang and Shih (2011) paper. A quiet
environment produced similar attention scores to instrumental in the Shi, Huang
and Chiang (2012) paper and better than instrumental in the Huang and Shih
(2011) paper. Also, participants whose preference for the background music was
‘dislike very much’ or ‘like very much’ scored lower on the attention test
(Huang & Shih, 2011).
Recall
tasks
Recall
tasks were not only correlated with music preference but also with the
personality of the listener. In Perham and Vizard’s (2011) research,
participants were exposed to 5 different music states – quiet, steady-state,
changing-state, liked music and disliked music ‒ as they tried to recall 25
different sets of 8 consonants. The steady-state was a man’s voice repeating the
same number and the changing-state was the same voice saying a sequence of
random digits (1-9). The liked music was the participants’ choice, all by
contemporary artists like Lady Gaga. The disliked music was thrash metal, which
participants agreed that they disliked in advance. They also filled out a
ratings questionnaire about the likeability, distractibility, offensiveness and
pleasantness of the sounds.
When participants heard quiet or steady-state
speech, they had more successful recall than when they heard changing-state
speech, liked or disliked music. They were less successful with music they
found likeable and pleasant, even offensive. Combining these results with the
findings of Huang and Shih (2011) show that both attention and recall tasks
scored less during liked and disliked music compared to a quiet environment.
The
distractibility property data collected by Perham and Vizard (2011) produced
interesting results. The musical conditions that participants found most
distracting (steady-state) and least distracting (quiet) produced similar
results. Participants were more successful with quiet and steady-state than
with music they liked, disliked or changing-state.
The
trend in the research discussed so far implies better success in quiet
environments but the following paper complicates matters as it associates the success
in quiet conditions with introverts, not extroverts. The research by Furnham and Allass (1999) was
based on Eysenck’s theory of personality which claimed that arousal levels
depended on personality such that introverts experienced arousal at lower-level
intensity stimulation than extroverts. Both simple and complex music used in
this study contained lyrics. Overall, extroverts performed better than
introverts in immediate recall tests. Extroverts performed their best during
complex background music but introverts performed their best during silence. To
the extroverts, there seemed no significant difference between hearing complex
or simple music in terms of distraction. To the introverts, however, complex
music was more distracting than simple music.
Comprehension
tasks
With
regard to reading comprehension, Furnham and Allass (1999) found that although
the trends were similar to the idea that extroverts perform better during
complex music and introverts during silence, the results were not significantly
different.
On
another language test, musicians and non-musicians were compared after they
marked grammatically incorrect sentences while they listened to silence, piano
music with no errors or piano music with errors (Patston & Tippett, 2011). Musicians
scored better than non-musicians overall in all categories. The musicians scored
less when there was piano music (compared to silence) and even worse when there
were errors in the music. Non-musicians were not impacted significantly by the three
different conditions. In visuospatial testing, musicians did better than non-musicians
overall but the difference within one group over the three different conditions
did not vary. In other words, the musicians were quite impacted during the
language tests by the change in conditions but not during the visuospatial
test. The authors suggest that the trend implies that the musical and language
processes overlap in a musicians brain so errors in one category (music) cause
the brain to make errors in the other category (language) and vice versa. Jourdain
(1997) suggests that expert listening would require more effort than passive
listening. Assuming that the musicians were familiar with the piano music,
information lacking in the paper, their brains would perhaps tend to analyse
deeper relations in the music and expend more energy in listening than
non-musicians especially when the anticipated relations were faulty.
As
evident in the research reviewed in this essay, it is quite difficult to make a
generalisation or recommendation with regard to listening to music as one is studying.
Not only is the type of task a determinant but also various qualities of the
music, our personality and our educational background. I also found that many
research papers discussed their results in terms of arousal, whereas Jourdain
(1997) maintained the importance of distinguishing between arousal and
attention. For example, no questions were asked to examine the attention given
to the music itself after the task tests other than likeability properties. A thorough
examination of the music used in addition to further insight by music experts
would be valuable to our understanding of the brain as it relates to music and
studying.
References
Furnham, A., &
Allass, K. (1999). The influence of musical distraction of varying complexity
on the cognitive performance of extroverts and introverts. European Journal
of Personality, 13(1), 27-38. doi:
10.1002/(SICI)1099-0984(199901/02)13:1<27::AID-PER318>3.0.CO;2-R
Jourdain, R.
(1997). Music, the brain, and ecstasy : How music captures our imagination
(1st ed. ed.). New York: W. Morrow.
Patston, L. L. M.,
& Tippett, L. J. (2011). The effect of background music on cognitive
performance in musicians and nonmusicians. Music Perception, 29(2),
173-183.
Perham, N., &
Vizard, J. (2011). Can preference for background music mediate the irrelevant
sound effect? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25(4), 625-631. doi:
10.1002/acp.1731
Huang, R.H. &
Shih, Y. (2011). Effects of background music on concentration of workers.
Work, 38(4), 383-387. doi: 10.3233/WOR-2011-1141
Shih, Y. N.,
Huang, R. H., & Chiang, H. Y. (2012). Background music: Effects on
attention performance. Work (Reading, Mass.), 42(4), 573-578. doi:
10.3233/WOR-2012-1410
2 comments:
I like the topic you choose for this paper as it covers a lot of ground concerning musicians, non-musicians and extroverts and introverts and the type of music we can tolerate while doing tasks. I know that I am an extrovert and sometimes listening to uplifting instrumental music helps me get rid of my inner critic especially when I am writing. I seem to focus more and have clearer thoughts. I was surprised to see that there have been no conclusive findings regarding this research.
Your essay highlights some very interesting ideas. I personally find it very difficult to focus when listening to music, especially if it is music I enjoy or have studied previously. I can definitely see how musicians in the Patston & Tippett study scored less when listening to piano music in comparison to silence. I thought it was interesting how musicians scored even worse the music had errors. I think I would react this way as well! It it also very interesting that non-musicians were not impacted as significantly as musicians in this study. My boyfriend, who has never studied music, can listen to music while he reads or studies - his brain is not trained to hear music in the same way musicians do. I suppose the reason I cannot study or read with music on has to do with the overlap of musical and language processes that you mention occur in musician's brains. I'd love to know more about how language and music overlap!
Post a Comment