Thursday, November 10, 2011

Images of Sonic Objects

Source:
Godøy, R. I. (2010, April). Images of sonic objects. Organised Sound, 15(1), 54-62. Cambridge University Press. Retrieved October 10, 2011, from Scholars Portal Journals
http://resolver.scholarsportal.info.myaccess.library.utoronto.ca/resolve/13557718/v15i0001/54_ioso

Summary:
Largely based on the theories of Pierre Schaeffer in his Traité des objets musicaux (1966), but also drawing on more recent evidence from the study of musical imagery and support from the theory of embodied cognition, Rolf Inge Godøy, Professor at the Department of Musicology, University of Oslo, argues that the “sonic object” is the most significant timescale of music with regard to human’s ability to form stable memory images of music (sonic images) from continuous sound.

First, Godøy gives some useful background information on musical imagery, which is defined as the “mental capacity for imagining musical sound in the absence of a directly audible sound source”. Placing musical imagery in the broader context of mental imagery, he explains that there is generally a “functional equivalence” between real-world perception and action and imagined perception and action. (For example, recalling the last verse of a song would take longer than the first verse because people usually scan through the song from the beginning.) Furthermore, neuroscientific research shows that mental imagery and real perception and action share much of the same neural substrate. Of particular interest in musical imagery is that auditory and motor imagery seem to be bidirectionally linked. (For example, when professional pianists listen to piano music, the motor areas of the brain are also activated. Vice versa, when the pianists see silent piano performance actions, they also mentally hear the music associated with those actions.) Then, putting musical imagery in the perspective of embodied cognition, which sees perception and cognition as intimately linked with sensations of movement, Godøy argues that body movements are integral to music and that sound-events should be “understood as included in some kind of gesture trajectory”.

All of the above background information helps to prepare the reader for Godøy’s ideas about the nature of sonic objects, which he defines as “holistically perceived fragments of sound, typically with durations in the 0.5 to 5 seconds range”. He justifies this timescale by citing research that shows that listeners can generally recognize salient musical features, such as style, rhythm, texture/timbre, modal/tonal features, and expressivity, within this 0.5 to 5 seconds range. He then points out that theories of memory support the idea of sonic objects as coherent chunks of sound that are perceived and imagined in the present moment (in a series of “now-points”). In this way, an entire piece of music is basically a chain of sonic objects perceived and imagined chunk-by-chunk, moment-by-moment. Godøy describes three types of sonic objects: 1) Impulsive, meaning abrupt attack followed by decay, 2) Sustained, and 3) Iterative, meaning a quick series of fluctuations (e.g. tremolo). Given the integral sound-gesture link in the embodied perspective, he remarks that the three types of sonic objects correlate well with impulsive, sustained, and iterative body gestures. And given the bidirectionality between motor and auditory imagery, Godøy believes the “kinematics and dynamics of sound-related actions can create images of sonic objects”, which carries the implication that action imagery can actually enhance musical imagery and, therefore, can potentially be applied in various contexts, such as musical practice, research, and education.

Reflection:
Though slightly difficult for me to digest, I still found this journal article quite fascinating. Having read a chapter titled “Imagined action, excitation, and resonance” by Godøy (2001) in a book called Musical imagery, which argues that “images of sound-producing actions… can enhance [the] capacity for imagining sonorous qualities” (p. 237), I was curious to find out if Godøy has written anything else on this subject more recently. As it turned out, he indeed has, and I chose this article because it offers more up-to-date information on musical imagery, a topic that I am deeply interested in.

First of all, I was not surprised at all to discover that auditory and motor imagery are linked; I can relate well to the experience of having the urge to move my fingers and “play along” when listening to other pianists performing pieces that I am acquainted with. Being a performer, I have absolutely no doubt that body movements are integral to musical experience. But Godøy’s suggestion that there is an important gestural component to sound would still have seemed a little strange to me had I not taken a course in conducting two years ago, which certainly made me much more aware of how gestures can accurately represent various sound qualities (with a lot of practice, of course).

What impressed me the most about this article was the fact that something as private and seemingly unobservable as imagery could be systematically studied and theorized upon so extensively. I think that Godøy backs up his argument about sonic objects convincingly. What I am primarily interested in, however, is whether action imagery would really prove effective in developing musical imagery in the context of mental practice, as his view implies. Up till now, I have rarely employed the strategy of mental practice myself. But I have always been taught that I must first know what kind of sound I want (in my “inner ear”) before I can experiment with various ways of pressing the keys that would get me closer to realizing that sound. So it seems to me that the music should come first and the action subservient to it. Nevertheless, I suppose that after some physical practice, the sound would become inseparable from the action associated with it, and, at this point, action imagery would be effective in bringing forth musical imagery. So perhaps one needs a certain amount of physical practice on a particular piece before action imagery can be used? Or maybe it would simply be best for one to start developing mental practice skills early on in one's training?

Reference
Godøy, R. I. (2001). Imagined action, excitation, and resonance. In R.I. Godøy, & H. Jørgensen (Eds.), Musical imagery (pp. 237-250). Exton, PA: Swets & Zeitlinger Publishers.

No comments: